La analogía funcional como estrategia de replicación de la información cultural

  1. José Henrique Pérez-Rodríguez
Journal:
El Genio Maligno: revista de humanidades y ciencias sociales

ISSN: 1988-3927

Year of publication: 2016

Issue Title: Espejos

Issue: 19

Type: Article

More publications in: El Genio Maligno: revista de humanidades y ciencias sociales

Abstract

Human cultural transmission exhibits a certain paradox: despite relying on an inferential process, it is characterized by having reached a relative degree of fidelity and therefore being able to experience cumulative evolutionary processes. Categorization and the use of conventionalized coordination devices as a means of social homogenization of information partially explain the relative success of humans in inferring others’ intentions. However, they do not give an account of how replicated information, besides being formed by discrete ‘pieces’ which are similar to the original ones, exhibits a supposedly similar or, at least, functionally equivalent self-organizing in each individual’s mind. A hypothesis is proposed according to which in information transmission a coordination of thought between the sender and the receiver with the aim of ensuring the generation of socially conventionalized cultural structures, would become particularly relevant. In turn, a possible role of this shared system in supporting the very processes of individual categorization is discussed, and a replication model based on a selective restriction on the receiver’s acquisition of information from the environment is proposed.

Bibliographic References

  • ABBOTT, B. (2008). Presuppositions and common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy, vol. 31, n. 5, p. 523-538.
  • ALTMANN, E. G., WHICHARD, Z. L. & MOTTER, A. E. (2013). Identifying trends in word frequency dynamics. Journal of Statistical Physics, vol. 151, n. 1-2, p. 277-288.
  • AUSUBEL, D. P. (2012) [2000]. The acquisition and retention of knowledge: a cognitive view, New York: Springer.
  • BLACKMORE, S. (1999). The Meme Machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • BRENNAN, S. (1998). The grounding problem in conversations with and through computers. En S. R. Fussell & R. J. Kreuz (eds.). Social and Cognitive Psychological Approaches to Interpersonal Communication. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 201–225.
  • CHRISTIN, P. A., Weinreich, D. M. & Besnard, G. (2010). Causes and evolutionary significance of genetic convergence. Trends in genetics, 26(9), 400-405.
  • CLARK, H. H. (1996). Communities, commonalities, and communication. Rethinking linguistic relativity, 17, 324-355.
  • (1996b). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • (2005). Coordinating with each other in a material world. Discourse Studies, vol. 7, n. 4-5, 507-525.
  • CLOAK, F. T. Jr. (1968). Is a cultural ethology possible? Research Previews, 13(2), 37-47.
  • (1975). Is a cultural ethology possible? Human Ecology, 3(3), 161-182.
  • CROFT, W. (2000). Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. Harlow: Pearson Education.
  • DAWKINS, R. (2006) [1976]. The Selfish Gene: 30th Anniversary edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • DANNA, K. (2014). The Study of Culture and Cognition. Sociological Forum, 29 (4), 1001-1006.
  • EVEN-ZOHAR, I. (1990) [1979]. Teoría del Polisistema (R. Bermudez Otero [trad.]. Polysystem Theory, Poetics Today, 11(1), 9-26). Tel Aviv: The Porter Institute for Poetics and Semiotics.
  • GARCÍA-LANDA, M. (1984). La teoría de la traducción y la psicología experimental de los procesos de percepción del lenguaje. Estudios de Psicología, 5 (1920), 173-193.
  • GARROD, S. & PICKERING, M. J. (2013). Dialogue: Interactive Alignment and Its Implications for Language Learning and Language Change. En P. M. Binder & K. Smith (eds.). The Language Phenomenon: Human Communication from Milliseconds to Millennia. New York: Springer Science & Business Media, 47-64.
  • GRICE, H. P. (1957). Meaning. The Philosophical Review, 66 (3), p. 377-388.
  • HALL, J. (2015). Rhododendron ponticum and its invasive hybrids. v. 1.0. En M. J. Gaywood et al. (eds.). The Species Action Framework Handbook. Perth: Scottish Natural Heritage.
  • HOCKETT, C. F. (1960) The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203: 88-96.
  • HUTH, A. G., DE HEER, W. A., GRIFFITHS, T. L., THEUNISSEN, F. E. & GALLANT, J. L. (2016). Natural speech reveals the semantic maps that tile human cerebral cortex. Nature, 532(7600), 453-458.
  • KRONFELDNER, M. (2007). Darwinism, memes, and creativity: A critique of Darwinian analogical reasoning from nature to culture. 311 f. Unpublished Thesis (PhD in Philosophy). Regensburg: University of Regensburg. [http://epub.uni-regensburg.de/10562/]
  • LEWIS, D. K. (1969). Convention: A Philosophical Study. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • LEWONTIN, R. C. (1970). The units of selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1, 1-18.
  • MARCUS, G. (2014, 11 de Julio). The Trouble with Brain Science. The New York Times, Opinion page. [http:// www.nytimes.com/2014/07/12/opinion/the-trouble-with-brain-science.html]
  • MCCROHON, L. (2012). The two-stage life cycle of cultural replicators. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum, 9, 149-170.
  • MCGREW, W. C. (2015). The cultured chimpanzee: nonsense or breakthrough? Human Ethology Bulletin, 30(1), 41-52.
  • MESOUDI, A. (2013). Studying cultural transmission within an interdisciplinary cultural evolutionary framework. En R. Ellen, S. J. Lycett & S. E. Johns (eds.). Cultural transmission: a critical anthropological synthesis. Oxford: Berghahn Books, 131-147.
  • MÜLLER, R. (2009). Language universals in the brain: How linguistic are they? Language Universals, 224-252.
  • MURPHY, G. L. (2004). The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • NEUBERT, A. & SHREVE, G. M. (1992). Translation as text. Kent/Londres: Kent State University Press.
  • NEVO, E. (1979). Adaptive convergence and divergence of subterranean mammals. Annual review of ecology and systematics, 10, 269-308.
  • NORD, C. (2005). Text analysis in translation: Theory, methodology, and didactic application of a model for translation-oriented text analysis. Amsterdam/New York: Rodopi.
  • PÄÄBO, S. (2015). The diverse origins of the human gene pool. Nature Reviews Genetics, 16 (6), 313-314.
  • PARK, S. Y., PETERSON, F. C., MOSQUNA, A., YAO, J., VOLKMAN, B. F. & CUTLER, S. R. (2015). Agrochemical control of plant water use using engineered abscisic acid receptors. Nature, 520(7548), 545-548.
  • PÉREZ-RODRÍGUEZ, J. H. (2014). La articulación de la expresión escrita. Ocnos: Revista de Estudios sobre Lectura, 12, 79-106.
  • (2015). Cultura, língua e ciclos de vida: manifestações e transmissão da informação cultural e linguística (Borrador enviado para publicación). [http://tinyurl.com/jhpr2015]
  • (2016). The role of categorization as a way of fidelity preservation in cultural transmission (Borrador enviado para publicación). [http://tinyurl.com/ jhpr2016a]
  • PFENNING, A. R. et al. (2014). Convergent transcriptional specializations in the brains of humans and song-learning birds. Science, 346(6215), 1256846.
  • PIAGET, J. 1976 [1945]. La formation du symbole chez l’enfant: imitation, jeu et rêve, image et representation. Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.
  • PICKERING, M. J. & GARROD, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169-225.
  • RAMSEY, G. (2013). Culture in humans and other animals. Biology & Philosophy, 28(3), 457-479.
  • REGATTIN, F. (2011). Qu’est-ce que la mémétique? Et quel rôle peut-elle jouer pour la traductologie? En Quaderni del CeSLiC. Occasional papers. Bologna: Centro di Studi Linguistico-Culturali (CeSLiC) y Alma Mater Studiorum (ALMADL).
  • RICHERSON, P. J. & BOYD, R. (2008) [2005]. Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
  • RITT, N. (2004). Selfish sounds and linguistic evolution: a Darwinian approach to language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • SAUSSURE, F. de (1995) [1916]. Cours de linguistique générale (ed. de Tullio de Mauro). Paris: Payot.
  • SCHRÖDINGER, E. (1992) [1944]. What is life? The physical aspect of the living cell, with mind and matter & autobiographical sketches. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • SHANNON, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. The Bell System Technical Journal, 27, 379-423, 623-656.
  • SPERBER, D. & WILSON, D. (1995) [1986]. Relevance: communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.
  • STALNAKER, R. (1973). Presuppositions. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 2, 447-457.
  • (2002). Common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy, 25(5-6), 701-721.
  • TOMASELLO, M. (2000). Culture and cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(2), 37-40.
  • & CARPENTER, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121-125.
  • KRUGER, A. C. & RATNER, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 16, 495-552.
  • TYLER, Tim (2011). Memetics: memes and the science of cultural evolution. Mersenne Publishing.
  • WIENER, N. (1948). Cybernetics or Control and communication in the animal and the machine. Cambridge, MA: Technology Press.
  • WILLIAMS, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  • (1992). Natural selection: domains, levels, and challenges. New York/Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • WILLNER, W. (2006). The association concept revisited. Phytocoenologia, 36(1), 67-76.
  • WILSON, E. O. (1978). On Human Nature. Harvard: Harvard University Press.